Available online 27 December 2014
Pharmacology of cognitive enhancers for exposure-based therapy of fear, anxiety and trauma-related disorders
- Under a Creative Commons license
Abstract
Pathological fear and anxiety are highly debilitating and, despite considerable advances in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy they remain insufficiently treated in many patients with PTSD, phobias, panic and other anxiety disorders. Increasing preclinical and clinical evidence indicates that pharmacological treatments including cognitive enhancers, when given as adjuncts to psychotherapeutic approaches [cognitive behavioral therapy including extinction-based exposure therapy] enhance treatment efficacy, while using anxiolytics such as benzodiazepines as adjuncts can undermine long-term treatment success. The purpose of this review is to outline the literature showing how pharmacological interventions targeting neurotransmitter systems including serotonin, dopamine, noradrenaline, histamine, glutamate, GABA, cannabinoids, neuropeptides (oxytocin, neuropeptides Y and S, opioids) and other targets (neurotrophins BDNF and FGF2, glucocorticoids, L-type-calcium channels, epigenetic modifications) as well as their downstream signaling pathways, can augment fear extinction and strengthen extinction memory persistently in preclinical models. Particularly promising approaches are discussed in regard to their effects on specific aspects of fear extinction namely, acquisition, consolidation and retrieval, including long-term protection from return of fear (relapse) phenomena like spontaneous recovery, reinstatement and renewal of fear. We also highlight the promising translational value of the preclinial research and the clinical potential of targeting certain neurochemical systems with, for example D-cycloserine, yohimbine, cortisol, and L-DOPA. The current body of research reveals important new insights into the neurobiology and neurochemistry of fear extinction and holds significant promise for pharmacologically-augmented psychotherapy as an improved approach to treat trauma and anxiety-related disorders in a more efficient and persistent way promoting enhanced symptom remission and recovery.
Keywords
- Fear extinction;
- Exposure therapy;
- Augmented relearning;
- Reconsolidation;
- Drug development;
- Cognitive enhancer
Abbreviations
- 5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine = serotonin;
- AC, adenylate cyclase;
- AMPA, α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid;
- AMY, amygdala;
- BA, basal amygdala;
- BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor;
- BLA, basolateral amygdaloid complex;
- BZD, benzodiazepine;
- CaMKII, Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II;
- cAMP, 3′-5′-cyclic adenosine monophosphate;
- Cav 1.2, L-type calcium channel alpha 1C isoform;
- Cav 1.3, L-type calcium channel alpha 1D isoform;
- CBT, cognitive behavioral therapy;
- CCK, cholecystokinin;
- CeA, central amygdala;
- CeM, centromedial amygdala;
- CeL, centrolateral amygdala;
- CNS, central nervous system;
- CREB, cAMP response element binding;
- CS, conditioned stimulus;
- DA, dopamine;
- DCS, D-cycloserine;
- DNA, deoxyribonucleic acid;
- eCB, endogenous cannabinoiod;
- ERK, extracellular regulated kinase;
- ERP, exposure and prevention CBT;
- FGF2, fibroblast growth factor-2;
- GABA, γ-aminobutyric acid;
- GABAA, GABAA receptor;
- GAD, generalized anxiety disorder;
- GAD (65/67), glutamate decarboxylase 65/67;
- GluN, NMDA receptor subtype;
- GR, glucocorticoid receptor;
- H3, histone H3 protein;
- H4, histone H4 protein;
- HAT, histone acetyltransferase;
- HDAC, histone deacetylase;
- HDAC2, histone deacetylase 2;
- HPA, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis;
- HPC, hippocampus;
- icv, intracerebroventricular;
- IL, infralimbic cortex;
- ITC, intercalated cell masses;
- K, lysine;
- KOR, kappa opioid receptor;
- LA, lateral amygdala;
- L-DOPA, levodopa;
- MAPK, mitogen-activated protein kinase;
- MDMA, 3,4-methylenedioxy-N-methylamphetamine;
- Mg2+, magnesium ion;
- mGluR, metabotropic glutamate receptor;
- miRNA, micro-RNA;
- mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex;
- mRNA, messenger ribonucleic acid;
- MS-275, entinostat (Pyridin-3-ylmethyl N-[[4-[(2-aminophenyl)carbamoyl] phenyl]methyl]carbamate);
- NMDA, N-methyl-D-aspartate;
- NMDAR, NMDA receptor;
- ns, not studied;
- NO, nitric oxide;
- NPS, neuropeptide S;
- NPY, neuropeptide Y;
- OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder;
- OXT, oxytocin;
- PAG, periaqueductal gray;
- PEPA, 4-[2-(Phenylsulphonylamino)ethylthio]-2,6-difluorophenoxyacetamide;
- PKC, as Ca2+ /phospholipid-dependent protein kinase C;
- PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder;
- PL, prelimbic cortex;
- SAD, social anxiety disorder;
- SAHA, vorinostat (N-hydroxy-N’-phenyl-octanediamide);
- SNP, single nucleotide polymorphism;
- SNRI, selective noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor;
- SSRI, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor;
- TrkB, tropomyosin-related kinase B;
- US, unconditioned stimulus;
- VGCC, voltage-gated calcium channel;
- VRET, virtual reality exposure;
- Zn2+, zinc ion
1. Introduction
Fear, anxiety and trauma-related disorders are associated with excessive fear reactions triggered by specific objects, situations or internal and external cues in the absence of any actual danger, and often include an inability to extinguish learned fear and to show adequate safety learning [(Michael et al., 2007; Wessa & Flor, 2007; Milad et al., 2009; Jovanovic et al., 2012; Milad et al., 2013) reviewed in (Holmes & Singewald, 2013) and (Kong et al., 2014)]. Pathological fear and anxiety occur in a range of psychiatric conditions, including various types of phobia (e.g. social phobia, agoraphobia or specific phobia), panic disorder with/without agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), generalized anxiety (GAD) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (ICD-10, 1994; DSM-5, 2013). These disorders comprise the most common mental disorders and are estimated to have a life-time prevalence of up to 28% amongst western populations (Kessler et al., 2005; Wittchen et al., 2011; Kessler et al., 2012). In addition to the personal suffering of patients, the economic burden caused by anxiety disorders is heavy (Gustavsson et al., 2011).
Available pharmacological and psychotherapeutic treatments (Bandelow et al., 2007) which aim to reduce fear and anxiety are associated with decreased symptom severity, but up to 40% of anxiety patients show only partial long-term benefit, and a majority of them fail to achieve complete remission (Hoffman & Mathew, 2008; Stein et al., 2009; Bandelow et al., 2012) clearly underlining the need for further improvement. Current pharmacological approaches either induce rapid anxiolytic effects (e.g. benzodiazepines, some antipsychotics) or require prolonged, chronic treatment (e.g. antidepressants) to attenuate symptoms of pathological fear and anxiety. Commonly employed psychotherapeutic interventions apply cognitive behavioral strategies and exposure techniques to help patients overcome the maladaptive beliefs and avoidance behaviors that reinforce the pathology related to fear-eliciting cues. Meta-analyses show that cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) does have efficacy for several anxiety disorders, including PTSD, but patients have difficulty bearing the demanding and exhausting process of therapy and many who do manage to cope with it respond only partially and often relapse with time (Choy et al., 2007).
One strategy to improve CBT is to augment psychotherapy with adjunctive pharmacological treatments. Early attempts at combining ‘CBT’ with anxiolytic medications [e.g. benzodiazepines (BZD)] showed that the combination was no more effective [in some instances even counterproductive (Marks et al., 1993; Wilhelm & Roth, 1997)] than psycho- or pharmacotherapy alone [for details see (Otto et al., 2010a; Otto et al., 2010b; Rodrigues et al., 2011; Dunlop et al., 2012; Hofmann, 2012)]. However, at least in some cases, this failure may have reflected idiosyncratic effects of the drugs tested (especially BZDs) rather than utility of the strategy itself, and there has been an intense search to identify agents that serve as more effective adjuncts to CBT. The preclinical assay most frequently used in this search is fear extinction – the focus of this current review. Extinction of fear following Pavlovian fear learning (Pavlov, 1927) in animals is procedurally similar to exposure-based CBT (Milad & Quirk, 2012). We will briefly outline different aspects of Pavlovian fear learning [(which is thought to be involved in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders, e.g. (Amstadter et al., 2009)] and extinction highlighting the key processes that could be targeted to augment fear extinction (see Figure 1 for an overview).
Fear and fear extinction
Experimentally, fear conditioning occurs when a previously neutral stimulus [conditioned stimulus (CS) – such as a tone or light-] is paired with an aversive, unconditioned stimulus (US – e.g. electric shock to the forearm in humans, mild foot shock in rodents), resulting in a CS-US association whereby the CS alone elicits a conditioned fear response (e.g. freezing in rodents or increased skin conductance in humans). Following a successful CS-US association, fear memories require consolidation, a process involving a cascade of molecular and cellular events that alter synaptic efficacy, as well as a prolonged systems level interaction between brain regions, to stabilize the memory (McGaugh, 2000). Once consolidated, fear memories, reactivated by presenting the CS, are de-stabilized to render the original fear memory liable to pharmacological/behavioral interference (see overview in Figure 1) and this is then followed by a second phase of molecular and cellular events to re-stabilize (re-consolidate) the (adapted) memory (Nader et al., 2000).
Fear memories can be attenuated by various processes and interventions (including pharmacological and psychological approaches), some producing temporary blunting of fear behaviors and others causing more long-lasting relief. Interfering with the re-consolidation by inhibiting molecular and cellular events supporting fear memory re-stabilization [Figure 1C, (Nader et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2006)], for example with ß-adrenoceptor blockers (Debiec & Ledoux, 2004; Kindt et al., 2009), has been proposed as a clinical approach to alleviating fear memories. For discussion on this and other means of reducing fear (e.g. via US habituation) we refer the reader to some excellent prior reviews (Graham et al., 2011; Schwabe et al., 2014). Other potential ways to relieve fear include safety learning (Rogan et al., 2005; Kong et al., 2014) and erasure-like mechanisms such as destruction of erasure-preventing perineuronal nets (Gogolla et al., 2009).
Alternatively, fear memories can also be extinguished. Fear extinction, a process originally described by Pavlov (Pavlov, 1927), entails repeated exposure to anxiety-provoking cues to establish a new memory that counters the original fear memory. The process is highly relevant to fear, anxiety and trauma-related disorders which are associated with negative emotional reactions triggered by specific objects, situations or internal and external cues that are excessive to the actual danger posed. Moreover, extinction in animals is procedurally similar to forms of CBT that rely on exposure to anxiety-provoking cues (see Figure 1) (Milad & Quirk, 2012), and anxiety disorders are associated with an inability to extinguish learned fear and to respond adequately to safety signals (Michael et al., 2007; Wessa & Flor, 2007; Milad et al., 2009; Jovanovic et al., 2012; Milad et al., 2013) [reviewed in (Holmes & Singewald, 2013)]. Thus, fear extinction has considerable translational utility. The key processes that can be targeted to pharmacologically augment fear extinction are summarized in Figure 1.
Extinction is a learning process driven by violation of the original CS=US contingency (termed ‘prediction error’ for review see (Pearce & Bouton, 2001; McNally & Westbrook, 2006)], but it also contains other elements including habituation and desensitization some also say erasure/destabilization (Lin et al., 2011). Some authors therefore favor the term ‘relearning’ over extinction [for discussion see (Riebe et al., 2012)]. As with fear learning, extinction occurs in two phases: extinction acquisition and extinction consolidation retrieval. The decrement in the fear response during ’extinction training’ is also termed ‘within-session extinction’. Similar to fear memory, stabilization of the extinction memory requires both a cascade of overlapping, but dissociable, molecular and cellular events [for review see (Myers & Davis, 2007; Orsini & Maren, 2012)] that alter synaptic efficacy and brain systems-level interactions (Pape & Pare, 2010). The strength of extinction memory can be assessed at some interval (usually >1 day) after extinction training in “extinction retrieval” sessions (also termed ‘extinction retention’ or ‘extinction expression’, but note that we use the term “extinction retrieval” throughout this review).
That extinction memories are prone to re-emergence (due to insufficient ‘longterm extinction’) indicates that the original fear memory is still in place and the extinction memory is weaker/more labile than the fear memory. This may be particularly true of older (‘remote’) fear memories (Tsai & Graff, 2014). The re-emergence of extinguished fear occurs under multiple circumstances: (i) renewal, when the CS is presented in a different context to that in which extinction training occurred; (ii) reinstatement, when the original US or another stressor as is given unexpectedly; and (iii) spontaneous recovery, when a significant period of time has elapsed following successful extinction training (Myers & Davis, 2007; Herry et al., 2010). The likelihood of fear re-emergence is dependent on the strength of the extinction memory which is also determined by the type of extinction protocol used see e.g. (Li & Westbrook, 2008; Laborda & Miller, 2013). Spontaneous recovery (Rowe & Craske, 1998b, a; Schiller et al., 2008), reinstatement (Schiller et al., 2008) and renewal (Effting & Kindt, 2007) are all observable in clinical settings, and can be readily exploited in the laboratory to identify drug- and other interventions that can prevent or fear re-emergence in animals and relapse in humans (Vervliet et al., 2013).
Our goal in the current review is to offer a comprehensive overview of preclinical work on the possible pharmacological approaches (see Box 1 for an overview) to augment fear extinction and protect against the re-emergence of fear, and to discuss the potential translational value of these candidates as adjuncts to exposure-based CBT in anxiety patients.
-
FACILITATING 5-HT SIGNALING
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE SERT KO6 ns – ns no drug (Hartley et al., 2012) no effect – ns no drug (Wellman et al., 2007) acute fluoxetine (SSRI) (-)## no effect ns ip (Lebron-Milad et al., 2013) subchronic
citalopram (SSRI, 9d)no effect ns ns ip (Burghardt et al., 2013) chronic citalopram (SSRI, 22d) – (-) ns ip (Burghardt et al., 2013) chronic (14d)
fluoxetine (SSRI)(+)# (+) ns po (Lebron-Milad et al., 2013) chronic Fluoxetine (SSRI) no effect + +
(Ren-A,
SR,
Re-in)ip (Karpova et al., 2011) no effect ns +
(Re-in)ip,po (Deschaux et al., 2011; Deschaux et al., 2013) no effect* +*4 ns po (Camp et al., 2012) no effect + ns po (Popova et al., 2014) acute venlafaxine (SSRI) no effect + ns ip (Yang et al., 2012) chronic venlafaxine (SSRI) ns ns +
(Re-in)ip (Yang et al., 2012) 8-OH-DPAT (5HT1 ag) +*5 (-)*5 ns ip (Wang et al., 2013) tandospirone (5HT1 ag) ns +* ns ip (Saito et al., 2013) tcb2 (5HT2A ag) + ns ns ip (Zhang et al., 2013) psilocybin (5HT2A ag) (+) ns ns ip (Catlow et al., 2013) 5-HT3 OE no effect ns ns no drug (Harrell & Allan, 2003) INHIBITING 5-HT SIGNALING 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine (+)# + ns BLA (Izumi et al., 2012) 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine no effect no effect ns IL (Izumi et al., 2012) 5HT3 KO ns – ns no drug (Kondo et al., 2014) MDL11,939 (5HT2A ant) – Ns ns ip (Zhang et al., 2013) ketanserin (5HT2A/C ant) (+) Ns ns ip (Catlow et al., 2013) granisetron (5HT3 ant) no effect (+) ns ip (Park & Williams, 2012) - 1drug administration following extinction training; 2 SERT-KO results in increased synaptic 5-HT levels; 3 7 days; 4 protection from context over-generalization; 5 valproate-induced model of autism, 6 SERT-KO results in increased synaptic 5-HT levels* facilitates rescue of impaired fear extinction; # reduced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training; ## enhanced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training
+, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; po, peroral administration; ip, intraperitoneal injection; ns, not studied; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; IL, infralimbic cortex; Ren-A, Fear renewal in conditioning context; SR, spontaneous recovery; Re-in, reinstatement; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out; OE, over-expression
-
Disorder Study Design Outcome (compared to placebo control group) Reference Healthy volunteers 14 days po escitalopram pretreatment; Fear conditioning paradigm Accelerated extinction learning (skin conductance responses) (Bui et al., 2013) Panic disorder with/without agoraphobia Fluvoxamine or placebo followed by exposure therapy; psychological panic management followed by exposure therapy or exposure therapy alone Self-reported measures
All treatments effective, however fluvoxamine plus CBT superior to all other treatments(de Beurs et al., 1995) Panic disorder with/without agoraphobia 12 weeks paroxetine or placebo plus CBT Reduced number of panic attacks in paroxetine/CBT group (Oehrberg et al., 1995) Panic disorder with/without agoraphobia 10 weeks of paroxetine or placebo plus CBT in week 5 and 7 No significant difference in primary CGI outcome.
Secondary outcome: Higher proportion of panic-free patients in paroxetine-CBT group.(Stein et al., 2000) Panic disorder with/without agoraphobia 12 weeks fluvoxamine or placebo with or without CBT All groups except placebo without CBT improved.
No difference within other groups.(Sharp et al., 1997) Panic disorder with/without agoraphobia 12 weeks of sertraline or placebo treatment plus self-administered CBT or no CBT Reduced anticipatory anxiety in
sertraline plus self-administered CBT
No significant improvements in CGI.(Koszycki et al., 2011) Social anxiety disorder 24 weeks of sertraline/ placebo with or without exposure therapy (8 sessions in the first 12 weeks of treatment) All groups improved (also placebo without CBT)
Sertraline treatment (without CBT) showed higher improvement than CBT groups (placebo or sertraline). Placebo without CBT showed lowest benefits.(Blomhoff et al., 2001) Social anxiety disorder Follow-up study of (Blomhoff et al., 2001)
Assessment of long-term effects 28 weeks after cessation of medical treatmentExposure therapy alone (without placebo or sertraline) showed a further improvement in CAPS 28 weeks after treatment cessation, however only reached improvement levels comparable with those of the sertraline alone group after the initial 24 weeks
CAPS score in the other groups (Exposure + placebo or sertraline and placebo alone) stayed constant (no improvement compared to 24 week CAPS score)(Haug et al., 2003) Social anxiety disorder 14 weeks of fluoxetine or placebo plus weekly CBT or no CBT
CBT consisted of group treatment combining in vivo exposure, cognitive restructuring and social skills trainingAll treatments were superior to placebo (without CBT) but no differences between groups themselves (Davidson et al., 2004) PTSD 10 exposure therapy sessions
(1x / week) plus paroxetine CR
Optional 12 weeks of maintenance treatmentGreater CAPS improvement in paroxetine group vs placebo after 10 weeks
Higher rate of remission (61.5% vs 23.1% in placebo group) after 10 weeks
No changes after additional 12 weeks. CAVE drop-out of remitters.(Schneier et al., 2012) - CR…controlled release
-
FACILITATING DA SIGNALING
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE L-DOPA ns +1 +
(SR, Re-in2
, Ren-Aip (Haaker et al., 2013) amphetamine – ns ns ip (Borowski & Kokkinidis, 1998) no effect ns ns ip (Carmack et al., 2010) (+)3 (-) ns ip (Mueller et al., 2009) cocaine – ns ns ip (Borowski & Kokkinidis, 1998) Methyl-
phenidate+ (+) ns ip (Abraham et al., 2012) ns +1 ns ip (Abraham et al., 2012) SKF38393
(pAg D1)– ns ns ip (Borowski & Kokkinidis, 1998) +⁎4 ns ns ip (Dubrovina & Zinov’eva, 2010) ns +1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns no effect1 ns BLA (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns no effect1 ns IL (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns +6 ns ip (Rey et al., 2014) ns –7 ns ip (Rey et al., 2014) Quinpirole
(D2 ag)ns – ns ip (Ponnusamy et al., 2005) ns – ns ip (Nader & LeDoux, 1999) –4 ns ns ip (Dubrovina & Zinov’eva, 2010) INHIBITING DA SIGNALING 6-OH DOPA – ns ns mPFC (Morrow et al., 1999) – (-) ns mPFC (Fernandez Espejo, 2003) D1 knock-out – ns ns global KO (El-Ghundi et al., 2001) SCH23390
(D1 ant)ns no effect1 ns IL (Fiorenza et al., 2012) – no effect ns BLA (Hikind & Maroun, 2008) no effect – ns IL (Hikind & Maroun, 2008) ns –1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns no effect1 ns BLA (Fiorenza et al., 2012) Sulpiride
(D2 ant)– ns ns ip (Dubrovina & Zinov’eva, 2010) ns + ns ip (Ponnusamy et al., 2005) no effect no effect ns ip (Mueller et al., 2010) Raclopride
(D2 ant)(-) no effect ns ip (Mueller et al., 2010) no effect – ns IL (Mueller et al., 2010) Haloperidol
(D2/D3 ant)– ns ns ip (Holtzman-Assif et al., 2010) (-) ns ns icv (Holtzman-Assif et al., 2010) (-) – ns NAcb (Holtzman-Assif et al., 2010) L-741,741
(D4 ant)no effect – ns mPFC (Pfeiffer & Fendt, 2006) - +, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; ip, intraperitoneal injection; ns, not studied; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; IL, infralimbic cortex; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; CA1, cornu ammonis 1; NAcb, nucleus accumbens; Ren-A, Fear renewal in conditioning context; SR, spontaneous recovery; Re-in, reinstatement; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out;
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training, 2 37 days, 3 increased locomotion, 4 passive avoidance paradigm, 5 reduced locomotion with 0.3mg/kg, no effect on locomotion, extinction training and retrieval with 0.1mg/kg, 6 rescued impaired extinction retrieval in estrus/metestrus/diestrus, 7 impairs extinction retrieval in proestrus
- ⁎
- facilitates rescue of impaired fear extinction; # reduced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training; ## enhanced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training
-
Disorder Study Design Outcome (compared to placebo control group) Reference Healthy volunteers Fear conditioning paradigm, L-DOPA po following extinction training Protection from renewal (skin conductance responses) (Haaker et al., 2013) PTSD (treatment-refractory ) 2 single oral MDMA prior
to 8h therapy session (3-5 weeks apart, patients also received therapy prior to first MDMA exposure as well as between and in follow-up)MDMA augmented CBT in comparison to placebo (83% vs 25% response)
(2 month follow-up); open-label MDMA use offered to patients from the placebo group after last assessment(Mithoefer et al., 2011) PTSD (treatment-refractory ) Follow-up study from (Mithoefer et al., 2011) MDMA augmented CBT produced long-lasting effects (17-74 months) (Mithoefer et al., 2013)
-
ENHANCING NORADRENERGIC SIGNALING
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION
LEARNINGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE noradrenaline ns + ns icv (Merlo & Izquierdo, 1967) methylphenidate + + ns ip (Abraham et al., 2012) noradrenaline ns +1 ns BLA (Berlau & McGaugh, 2006) ns no effect1 ns BLA (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns no effect1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns –1 ns IL (Fiorenza et al., 2012) atomoxetine (NE reuptake inhibitor) ns no effect6 +
(SR)5systemic (Janak & Corbit, 2011) isoproterenol (β ag) no effect ns ns ip (subchronic) (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) ns (+)1 ns ip (subchronic) (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) ns (+)1 ns mPFC (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) yohimbine (α2 ant)8 + ns ns sc (Cain et al., 2004) ns no effect1 ns sc (Cain et al., 2004) +#2 +2 ns sc (Cain et al., 2004) + + – ip (Morris & Bouton, 2007) no effect7 no effect ns ip (Mueller et al., 2009) ns +* ns ip (Hefner et al., 2008) ns no effect6 +
(SR)5systemic (Janak & Corbit, 2011) atipamezole (α2 ant)8 ns no effect4 ns sc (Davis et al., 2008) INHIBITING NORADRENERGIC SIGNALING prazosine (α1 ant) ns no effect1 ns ip (subchronic) (Do-Monte et al., 2010a) – ns ns ip (subchronic) (Do-Monte et al., 2010a) – ns ns intra-mPFC (Do-Monte et al., 2010a) propanolol (β ant) no effect ns ns sc (Cain et al., 2004) ns no effect1 ns sc (Cain et al., 2004) no effect2 (-)3 ns sc (Cain et al., 2004) (+)# no effect ns ip (Rodriguez-Romaguera et al., 2009) – – ns ip (subchronic) (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) ns no effect1 ns ip (subchronic) (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) atenolol (β ant) – ns ns mPFC (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) ns no effect1 ns mPFC (Do-Monte et al., 2010b) sotalol (β ant) no effect no effect ns ip (Rodriguez-Romaguera et al., 2009) timolol (β ant) ns +1 ns BLA (Fiorenza et al., 2012) propanolol (β ant) no effect – ns IL (Mueller et al., 2008) ns no effect1 ns IL (Mueller et al., 2008) timolol (β ant) ns +1 ns IL (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns no effect1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) - * facilitates rescue of impaired fear extinction; ** facilitates extinction of remote memories, *** only in older animals (7 months), younger ones (2 months) are not affected, # reduced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training+, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; ip, intraperitoneal injection; injection; sc, subcutanous injection; icv, intracerebroventricular injection; ns, not studied; HPC, intra-hippocampal administration; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex; CA1, cornu ammonis 1; Ren, Fear renewal; SR, spontaneous recovery; Re-in, reinstatement; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out; #, reduced fear expression at start of extinction training
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training; 2 spaced CS extinction, US=0.7mA; 3 spaced CS extinction when US=0.4mA; no effect when US=0.7mA, 4 cocaine-conditioned place preference, 5 4 weeks, 6 instrumental lever press response, 7 reduced fear expression at start of extinction training, if extinction is performed in the same context as conditioning, 8 note that the net effect of α2 blockade is enhancement of noradrenergic signaling (see text)
-
Disorder Study Design Outcome (compared to placebo control group) Reference Social anxiety disorder (SAD) 4 sessions consisting of yohimbine being administered prior to a CBT asession Yohimbine augmented CBT-induced improvement in SAD (self-report measures; 21 day follow-up) (Smits et al., 2014) Claustrophobic fear 1 single oral yohimbine dose prior to an exposure b session Yohimbine augment CBT-induced reduced fear of enclosed spaces (7 day follow-up) (Powers et al., 2009) Fear of flying 2 single oral yohimbine prior to VRET session No fear augmenting effect of yohimbine c (Meyerbroeker et al., 2012) - VRET, virtual reality exposure.
-
- a
- CBT involved an oral presentation on challenging topics in front of the therapists, other group members, and confederates,
- b
- behavioral approach task,
- c
- lack of yohimbine effect might possibly have been due to the powerful influence of VRET exposure itself leading to a greater impact on the results from this than from the manipulation (drug condition) that ‘washed out’ or overrode any effect of manipulation (Meyerbroeker et al., 2012),
-
FACILITATING AMPA SIGNALING
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE PEPA (AMPA potentiator) no effect⁎ no effect⁎ ns ip (Whittle et al., 2013) no effect⁎ +⁎ ns ip (Yamada et al., 2011) +# + +
(Re-in)ip (Yamada et al., 2009) + + ns ip (Zushida et al., 2007) PEPA + NBQX (AMPA ant) no effect no effect ns ip (Zushida et al., 2007) PEPA (AMPA potentiator) (+) (+) ns PL (Zushida et al., 2007) + + ns BLA/ceA (Zushida et al., 2007) INHIBITING AMPA SIGNALING CNQX
(AMPA ant)ns no effect ns BLA (Falls et al., 1992; Lin et al., 2003b; Zimmerman & Maren, 2010) FACILITATING NMDA SIGNALING D-cycloserine ns + ns ip (Walker et al., 2002) ns +1 ns systemic (Ledgerwood et al., 2003) ns +1 +
(Re-in)systemic (Ledgerwood et al., 2004) ns +1 ns systemic (Ledgerwood et al., 2005) ns +1 ns ip (Parnas et al., 2005) ns + ns ip (Yang & Lu, 2005) ns + ns ip (Lee et al., 2006) ns + no effect sc (Woods & Bouton, 2006) ns + ns sc (Werner-Seidler & Richardson, 2007) ns + ns systemic (Weber et al., 2007) ns + ns ip (Yang et al., 2007) ns + ns ip (Yang et al., 2007) ns + ns ip (Hefner et al., 2008) ns + ns po (Yamamoto et al., 2008) ns + ns ip (Matsumoto et al., 2008) + + ns ip (Silvestri & Root, 2008) ns + ns sc (Langton & Richardson, 2008) ns + no effect sc (Bouton et al., 2008) ns + ns ip (Lin et al., 2010) +# + +
(Re-in)ip (Yamada et al., 2009) + ns ns ip (Lehner et al., 2010) ns +1 ns systemic (Langton & Richardson, 2010) no effect⁎ +⁎ ns ip (Yamada et al., 2011) no effect + ns ip (Toth et al., 2012b) ns +1 ns ip (Toth et al., 2012b) ns + ns systemic (Gupta et al., 2013) ns + ns ip (Matsuda et al., 2010) + + ns ip (Bai et al., 2014) ns +1⁎ ns ip (Whittle et al., 2013) ns + ns BLA (Walker et al., 2002) ns +1 ns BLA (Ledgerwood et al., 2003) ns + ns BLA (Lee et al., 2006) ns + ns BLA (Mao et al., 2008) ns + ns BLA (Lee et al., 2006) ns + ns BLA (Akirav et al., 2009) ns no effect ns BLA (Bolkan & Lattal, 2013) ns + ns HPC (Bolkan & Lattal, 2013) ns + ns HPC (Ren et al., 2013) spermidine ns +1 ns HPC (Gomes et al., 2010) INHIBITING NMDA SIGNALING MK-801 (non-competitive NMDA ant) ns – ns systemic (Baker & Azorlosa, 1996; Storsve et al., 2010) ns ns +
(Re-in)sc (Johnson et al., 2000) ns –1 ns ip (Lee et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2009) ns – ns systemic (Langton et al., 2007; Chan & McNally, 2009) CPP (competitive NMDA ant) no effect –5 ns ip (Santini et al., 2001; Sotres-Bayon et al., 2007) no effect – ns mPFC (Burgos-Robles et al., 2007) ns –1 ns mPFC (Burgos-Robles et al., 2007) – – ns BLA (Parsons et al., 2010) AP-5 (competitive NMDA ant) ns – ns BLA (Falls et al., 1992) –## – ns BLA (Lee & Kim, 1998) ns –1 ns BLA (Lin et al., 2003a; Laurent et al., 2008) (Fiorenza et al., 2012) no effect – ns BLA (Lin et al., 2003a; Zimmerman & Maren, 2010) ns – ns CA1 (Szapiro et al., 2003) ns –1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns –1 ns mPFC (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ifenprodil (non-comp NR2B-NMDA ant) – – ns ip (Sotres-Bayon et al., 2007) ns –1 ns systemic Sotres-Bayon et al., 2009 – – ns BLA (Sotres-Bayon et al., 2007; Laurent et al., 2008; Laurent & Westbrook, 2008) ns no effect1 ns BLA (Laurent et al., 2008; Laurent & Westbrook, 2008; Sotres-Bayon et al., 2009) no effect – ns mPFC (Laurent & Westbrook, 2008) no effect no effect ns mPFC (Sotres-Bayon et al., 2009) no effect –1 ns mPFC (Laurent & Westbrook, 2008; Sotres-Bayon et al., 2009) ns –1 ns HPC (Gomes et al., 2010) Ro25-6981 (non-comp NR2B-NMDA ant) – no effect ns ip (Dalton et al., 2008; Dalton et al., 2012) FACILITATING mGluR SIGNALING CDPPB (mGluR5 pos modulator) +3 ns ns sc (Gass & Olive, 2009) AMN082 (mGluR7 ag) +⁎ +⁎ ns ip (Whittle et al., 2013) ns + ns po (Fendt et al., 2008) – no effect ns ip (Toth et al., 2012b) ns –1 ns ip (Toth et al., 2012b) (+) no effect ns BLA (Dobi et al., 2013) – – ns mPFC (Morawska & Fendt, 2012) INHIBITING mGluR SIGNALING CPCCOEt (non-comp mGluR1 ant) – – ns BLA (Kim et al., 2007) no effect no effect no effect
(SR)ip (Mao et al., 2013) mGluR4 KO ns + ns no drug (Davis et al., 2013) mGluR5 KO – – ns no drug (Xu et al., 2009) MTEP (mGluR5 ant) no effect – –
(SR)ip (Mao et al., 2013) ns ns –
(SR)BLA (Mao et al., 2013) MPEP (allosteric mGluR5 ant) no effect no effect ns ip (Toth et al., 2012b) ns no effect1 ns ip (Toth et al., 2012b) no effect – ns ip (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011) no effect – ns IL (Fontanez-Nuin et al., 2011) – – ns IL (Sepulveda-Orengo et al., 2013) mGluR7 KO –4 ns ns no drug (Callaerts-Vegh et al., 2006; Goddyn et al., 2008) + + ns no drug (Fendt et al., 2013) siRNA knock-down of mGluR7 –2 ns ns no drug (Fendt et al., 2008) mGluR8 KO no effect# no effect ns no drug (Fendt et al., 2013) (S)-3,4-DCPG (mGluR8 ag) – no effect ns BLA (Dobi et al., 2013) - +, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; po, peroral administration; ip, intraperitoneal injection; sc, subcutaneous injection; ns, not studied; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; HPC, hippocampus; IL, infralimbic cortex; SR, spontaneous recovery; Re-in, reinstatement; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out;
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training; 2conditioned taste aversion; 3 cocaine conditioned place preference; 4 food-rewarded operant conditioning, 5 reduced locomotion
- ⁎
- facilitates rescue of impaired fear extinction; # reduced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training; ## enhanced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training
-
Disorder Study Design Outcome (compared to placebo control group) Reference Healthy volunteers DCS or placebo 2-3h prior extinction training No effect on fear extinction or fear recovery in healthy volunteers (Guastella et al., 2007) Healthy volunteers DCS, placebo, valproic acid or combination of DCS and valproic acid 1.5h prior extinction training Administration of DCS, valproic acid or DCS/valproic acid combination facilitates fear extinction and protects from reinstatement (Kuriyama et al., 2011) Healthy volunteers DCS or placebo 2h prior extinction training No effect on extinction learning and retention in healthy volunteers (Klumpers et al., 2012) Acrophobia DCS or placebo 1h prior 2 sessions of virtual reality exposure
3 month follow-upreduced fear symptoms in DCS group at all timepoints (Ressler et al., 2004) Acrophobia 2 sessions virtual reality exposure plus DCS or placebo after the sessions
1 month follow-up
Re-evaulation of Tart et al., 2013no advantage of DCS over placebo augmented VRE
DCS effect depends on success of exposure sessions (within-session fear reduction)(Tart et al., 2013)
(Smits et al., 2013a)Snake phobia DCS or placebo 1h prior a single exposure session same level of improvement with DCS, but DCS group achieved fear reduction quicker (Nave et al., 2012) Panic disorder DCS or placebo 1h prior CBT session 3-5
1 month follow-upDCS group showed greater reduction in panic symptom severity at all timepoints (Otto et al., 2010c) Panic disorder with agoraphobia DCS or placebo 1h prior 3 individual exposure sessions + 8 group CBT sessions
5 months follow-upno benefits of DCS at all timepoints, but initial benefical effects in severely symptomatic patients? (Siegmund et al., 2011) PTSD DCS or placebo 1h prior 10 weekly exposure sessions No overall enhancement of treatment effects.
Higher symptom reduction in severe cases.(de Kleine et al., 2012) PTSD
(combat-related)DCS or placebo 30min prior exposure session 2-6 Weaker symptom reduction compared to placebo group (Litz et al., 2012) PTSD DCS or placebo 1.5h prior 12 weekly VRE session
6 months follow-upDCS group showed earlier and greater improvement as well as higher remission rates (Difede et al., 2014) pediatric PTSD DCS or placebo 12 1h prior session 5-12 No difference in symptom reduction, DCS group showed trend for faster response and better retention in 3 month follow-up (Scheeringa & Weems, 2014) SAD 1x psychoeducation; DCS or placebo 1h prior to 4h exposure session (5x, individual or group)
1 month follow-upDecreased self-reported social anxiety symptoms in DCS group after 1 month (Hofmann et al., 2006) SAD DCS or placebo 1h prior to 4h exposure therapy (5x) Fewer social fear and avoidance in DCS group. Significant differences in DCS vs placebo following 3rd exposure session. (Guastella et al., 2008) SAD DCS or placebo with CBT Greater overall rates of improvement and lower post-treatment severity (Smits et al., 2013b) SAD 12 weeks; DCS or placebo 1h prior 5 exposure sessions (12 sessions at all); 6-months follow-up similar response and remission rates, DCS group improved quicker (Hofmann et al., 2013b) OCD D-cycloserine (DCS) or placebo 4h prior each exposure session, 12 weeks (one exposure session/week) No statistically significant difference.
High response rates in treatment and placebo group.(Storch et al., 2007) OCD 2x/week DCS or placebo 2h prior exposure and response prevention (ERP) sessions
(max 10)
3 months follow-upFaster improvements in DCS group
No difference between DCS and placebo group (no further benefit).(Kushner et al., 2007) OCD 2x/week DCS or placebo 1h prior 10 ERP sessions
1 month follow-up
Re-evaluation of Wilhelm et al., 2008Faster improvements in DCS group
No difference between DCS and placebo group (no further benefit).
specific improvements of DCS in the first 5 sessions, 6x faster than placebo group(Wilhelm et al., 2008)
(Chasson et al., 2010)Pediatric OCD DCS or placebo 1h prior weekly session 4-10 (psychoeduction, cognitive training and ERP) for 10 wks modest reduction of obsessive symptoms (Storch et al., 2010) Pediatric OCD DCS or placebo 1h prior session 5-9 ERP-CBT Significant improvements in OCD severity from posttreatment to 1-month follow-up in severe and difficult-to-treat pediatric OCD (Farrell et al., 2013) Pediatric OCD DCS or placebo immediately after each of 10 CBT (ERP) sessions
1 year follow-upboth groups improved;
no significant advantage of DCS at any timepoint(Mataix-Cols et al., 2014)
-
FACILITATING GABA SIGNALING
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE GABAA Agonists GABA-binding site (αβ interface) muscimol +# no effect ns BLA (Akirav et al., 2006) ns +1,7 ns BLA (Akirav et al., 2006) –# – ns BLA (Laurent et al., 2008; Laurent & Westbrook, 2008) ns –1 ns BLA (Laurent & Westbrook, 2008) (+)# – ns BLA (Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011) – – ns mPFC (Laurent & Westbrook, 2008) ns –1 ns mPFC (Laurent & Westbrook, 2008) ns no effect1, ns IL (Akirav et al., 2006) +# (+) ns IL (Akirav et al., 2006) – – ns IL (Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011) no effect – ns IL (Laurent & Westbrook, 2009) (+)# no effect ns PL (Laurent & Westbrook, 2009; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011) ns ns +
(Ren-A)8dHPC (Corcoran et al., 2005) – – no effect
(Ren-A)dHPC (Corcoran et al., 2005) (+)# – ns vHPC (Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011) GABAA: Agonists BZD-binding site αγ interface) chlordiazepoxide – ns ns ip (Goldman, 1977) ns – ns ip (Bouton et al., 1990) diazepam ns – ns ip (Pereira et al., 1989) ns – ns ip (Bouton et al., 1990) midazolam ns –1 ns ip (Bustos et al., 2009) –##6 – ns ip (Hart et al., 2009, 2010; Hart et al., 2014) –#6 – ns BLA (Hart et al., 2009, 2010; Hart et al., 2014) GABAB receptor agonists baclofen ns – ns ip (Heaney et al., 2012) GS39783 (positive modulator GABAB) ns no effect ns po (Sweeney et al., 2013) INHIBITING GABA SIGNALING DRUG EXTINCTION
LEARNINGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE GAD67 KD in BLA – ns ns no drug (Heldt et al., 2012) GAD65 KO –2 –2 ns no drug (Sangha et al., 2009) α5 KD in HPC ns –3 ns no drug (Yee et al., 2004) α1 KO in CRF+ cells – – ns no drug (Gafford et al., 2012) picrotoxin ns + ns ip (McGaugh et al., 1990) ns +4 ns IL (Thompson et al., 2010) ns +4 ns PL (Thompson et al., 2010) ns +* ns (Fitzgerald et al., 2014b) bicuculline ns +1 ns BLA (Berlau & McGaugh, 2006) bicuculline + propanolol ns no effect1 ns BLA (Berlau & McGaugh, 2006) FG7142 – – ns ip (Harris & Westbrook, 1998) ns – ns ip (Kim & Richardson, 2007, 2009) GABAB receptor antagonism GABAB(1b) KO –5 ns ns no drug (Jacobson et al., 2006) phaclofen ns no effect ns ip (Heaney et al., 2012) CGP52432 ns no effect ns po (Sweeney et al., 2013) - 1 drug administration following extinction training; 2 only cued memory, contextual intact, 3 trace conditioning, 4 reconsolidation (injection prior reactivation of memory), 5 conditioned taste aversion, 6 midazolam may not impair fear extinction if initial extinction occurs drug-free [see (Hart et al., 2014)], 7 short extinction training (5 CS presentations), 8 injection prior renewal-testing# reduced fear expression at start of extinction training; ## enhanced immobility at start of extinction training;
+, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; ip, intraperitoneal injection; po,peroral administration; ns, not studied; HPC, intra-hippocampal administration; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex; Ren-A, Fear renewal in conditioning context; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out;
-
ENHANCED CANNABINOID SIGNALING
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTNCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM EXTNCTION ROUTE REFERENCE cannabidiol + + ns icv (Bitencourt et al., 2008) + + ns IL (Do Monte et al., 2013) anandamide ns +1 ns dCA1 (de Oliveira Alvares et al., 2008) AM404
(eCB uptake inhibitor)ns + ns ip (Pamplona et al., 2008) ns + +
(Re-in)ip (Chhatwal et al., 2005b) + (+) ns icv (Bitencourt et al., 2008) no training + ns IL (Lin et al., 2009) AM3506
(FAAH
Inhibitor)ns + ns ip (Gunduz-Cinar et al., 2013b) WIN 55,212-2
(low-dose CB1 Ag)+⁎ no effect2 ns ip (Pamplona et al., 2006) WIN 55,212-2
(high-dose CB1 Ag)(-)⁎ no effect2 ns ip (Pamplona et al., 2006) WIN 55,212-2
(CB1 Ag)+° +° ns ip (Pamplona et al., 2006) ns + nd ip (Pamplona et al., 2008) WIN 55,212-2 (CB1 Ag) ns +4 ns BLA (Ganon-Elazar & Akirav, 2013) ns +4 ns HPC (Ganon-Elazar & Akirav, 2013) ns no effect4 ns mPFC (Ganon-Elazar & Akirav, 2013) ns + no effect
(Re-in, SR3)IL (Lin et al., 2009) no training + ns IL (Lin et al., 2009) ns no effect ns PL (Kuhnert et al., 2013) HU210 no training + ns IL (Lin et al., 2009) URB597 (AEA hydrolysis
inhibitor)no training + ns IL (Lin et al., 2009) AM3506 (FAAH
Inhibitor)
+ SR141716A (CB1 Ant)ns no effect ns ip + BLA (Gunduz-Cinar et al., 2013b) AM3506
(FAAH
Inhibitor)ns + ns BLA (Gunduz-Cinar et al., 2013b) no training no effect ns BLA (Gunduz-Cinar et al., 2013b) cannabidiol
+ SR141716A (CB1 Ant)no effect no effect ns IL + ip (Do Monte et al., 2013) REDUCED CANNABINOID SIGNALING DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTNCTION
LEARNINGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONADMINISTRATIONROUTE REFERENCE CB1 KO – – ns no drug (Cannich et al., 2004) CB1 KO – ns ns no drug (Marsicano et al., 2002; Kamprath et al., 2006; Dubreucq et al., 2010; Plendl & Wotjak, 2010) D1-CB1 KO – ns ns no drug (Terzian et al., 2011) SR141716A
CB1 antns no effect1 ns sc (Marsicano et al., 2002) – ns ns sc, ip (Kamprath et al., 2006; Niyuhire et al., 2007; Plendl & Wotjak, 2010) ns –1 ns ip (Suzuki et al., 2004) – no effect ns ip (Pamplona et al., 2006; Pamplona et al., 2008) ns – ns ip (Chhatwal et al., 2005b) –## no effect ns ip (Bowers & Ressler, 2014) AM251
(CB1 inverse
ag)– ns ns ip (Reich et al., 2008) ns –1 ns dCA1 (de Oliveira Alvares et al., 2008) ns – ns IL (Lin et al., 2009) no training no effect ns IL (Lin et al., 2009) ns – ns PL (Kuhnert et al., 2013) - +, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; ip, intraperitoneal injection; ip, sc, subcutanous injection; ns, not studied; HPC, intra-hippocampal administration; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex; CA1, cornu ammonis 1; Re-in, reinstatement; SR, spontaneous recovery, ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out;
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training; 2 drug-free retrieval of contextual fear memory; 3 5 days, 4 single-prolonged-stress model: injection immediately after trauma
- ⁎
- , Recent memory; °, remote memory; ##, enhanced immobility at start of extinction training;
-
Disorder Study Design Outcome (compared to placebo control group) Reference Healthy volunteers Fear conditioning paradigm, Cannabidiol inhalation prior or following extinction Reduced fear in retrieval
Protection against reinstatement(Das et al., 2013) Healthy volunteers Fear conditioning paradigm, tetrahydro-cannabinol prior extinction No effect (Klumpers et al., 2012) Healthy volunteers Fear conditioning paradigm,
Dronabinol prior extinctionReduced fear in retrieval (Rabinak et al., 2013)
-
NEUROPEPTIDES
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE oxytocin – ns ns icv (Toth et al., 2012a) +# + ns DLS (Zoicas et al., 2014) -*5 -*5 ns ip (Eskandarian et al., 2013) chronic oxytocin (30d) ns no effect*6 ns in (Bales et al., 2014) NPY + + ns icv (Gutman et al., 2008) NPY KO –3⁎⁎⁎ – ns no drug (Verma et al., 2012) NPY Y1 KO – no effect ns no drug (Verma et al., 2012) NPY Y2 KO no effect no effect ns no drug (Verma et al., 2012) NPY Y1/Y2 KO – – ns no drug (Verma et al., 2012) Leu31Pro-NPY
(Y1 ag)ns + ns BLA (Lach & de Lima, 2013) BIBO 3304 (Y1 ant) ns – ns BLA (Gutman et al., 2008) NPS (+)4 no effect no effect BLA (Jungling et al., 2008) + ns ns icv (Slattery et al., in press) SHA68 (NPS-R ant) –4,⁎⁎⁎ – –
(Ren-A)BLA (Jungling et al., 2008) naloxone – – ns systemic (McNally & Westbrook, 2003) ns no effect1 ns systemic (McNally & Westbrook, 2003) – – ns vlPAG (McNally et al., 2004) – – ns vlPAG (Parsons et al., 2010) no effect no effect ns BLA (Parsons et al., 2010) CTAP (MOR ant) – – ns vlPAG (McNally et al., 2005) dynorphin KO – 3 – ns no drug (Bilkei-Gorzo et al., 2012) nor-BNI (KOR ant) ns –1 ns systemic (Bilkei-Gorzo et al., 2012) - +, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; ip, intraperitoneal injection; icv, intra-cerebroventricular injection; ns, not studied; HPC, intra-hippocampal administration; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; DLS, dorsolateral septum; vlPAG, ventrolateral periaqueductal gray; Ren-A, fear renewal in conditioning context; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out;
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training; 2 social fear conditioning;3 animals show accelerated fear learning; 4 administration 2h prior extinction training; 5 single prolonged stress model; 6 BTBR mouse autism model and C57BL6J
- ⁎⁎⁎
- enhanced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training, # reduced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training
-
FACILITATING GLUCOCORTICOID SIGNALING
DRUG EXTINCTION TRAINING EXTINCTION RETRIEVAL LONG-TERM EXTINCTION ROUTE REFERENCE corticosterone (GR/MR ag) ns +1 no effect
(Re-in)ip (Blundell et al., 2011) ns –2 ns ip (Den et al., 2014) dexamethasone (GR ag) ns + ns ip (Yang et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2007; Ninomiya et al., 2010) RU28362 (GR ag) ns + ns AMY (Yang et al., 2006) ganoxolone (allopregnanolone analog) +*3 +*3 ns ip (Pinna & Rasmusson, 2014) INHIBITING GLUCOCORTICOID SIGNALING metyrapone (CORT/cortisol synthesis inhibitor) no effect – -* sc (Barrett & Gonzalez-Lima, 2004; Clay et al., 2011) ns – ns sc (Yang et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2007; Blundell et al., 2011) mifepristone (GR and progesterone ant) ns – ns AMY (Yang et al., 2006) - +, improved; -, impaired; ip, intraperitoneal injection; sc, subcutaneous injection; ns, not studied; AMY, intra-amygdala administration; Re-in, reinstatement; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist; GR, glucocorticoid receptor; MR, mineralocorticoid receptor; CORT, corticosteroid
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training; 2 in adolescent but not adult rats if exposed to CORT one week prior to (drugfree) testing; adults also show impaired extinction retrieval if they were exposed to one week of CORT in their adolescence; 3 socially isolated mice, CAVE ganoxolone was administered following a reactivation session 24h prior extinction training
-
Disorder Study Design Outcome (compared to placebo control group) Reference Social phobia Cortisone or placebo administered orally 1 h before a socio-evaluative stressor Reduced self-reported fear during anticipation and exposure (Soravia et al., 2006) Spider phobia Cortisol or placebo administered orally 1 h before exposure to a spider photograph in 6 sessions distributed over 2 weeks Progressive reduction of stimulus-induced fear
Reduction of fear was maintained also 2 days following session ending
(OFF-drug)(Soravia et al., 2006) Spider phobia Cortisol or placebo administered orally 1 h before 2 exposure therapy sessions
Follow-up after 1 monthSignificant decrease in phobic symptoms assessed with the Fear of Spiders Questionnaire
Cortisol-treated patients reported significantly less anxiety during exposure to living spiders at follow-up(Soravia et al., 2014) Acrophobia Cortisol or placebo was administered orally 1 h before virtual reality exposure to heights
Follow-up after 1 monthGreater reduction in fear of heights measured with the acrophobia questionnaire both at post-treatment and at follow-up (de Quervain et al., 2011) PTSD (combat-related ) Memory reactivation task followed by intravenous administration of cortisol or saline
Follow-up after 1 monthReduced PTSD symptomatic in cortisol-treated patients
No differences between cortisol and saline treated patients (initial improvement was lost after 1 month)(Suris et al., 2010) PTSD 10 weekly sessions of prolonged exposure therapy, cortisol or placebo 30min prior session
3-10Accelerated and greater decline in PTSD symptoms
Caveat – includes only 2 patients
(1 cortisol / 1 placebo)(Yehuda et al., 2010)
-
NEUROTROPHINS
DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION LEARNING EXTINCTION RETRIEVAL LONG-TERM EXTINCTION ROUTE REFERENCE FGF2 (+)# + ns sc (Graham & Richardson, 2009) ns +1 +
(Re-in)
(Ren-A)sc (Graham & Richardson, 2009, 2010) ns +1 +
(Ren-A)BLA (Graham & Richardson, 2011b) BDNF no training +**2 no effect (Re-in) IL (Peters et al., 2010; Rosas-Vidal et al., 2014a) no training no effect2 ns PL (Rosas-Vidal et al., 2014a) 7.8-dihydroxyflavone (+)* no effect (+)
(Re-in)ip (Andero et al., 2011) no effect no effect* +
(Ren-A)ip (Baker-Andresen et al., 2013) Lentiviral transfected dominant negative form of TrkB no effect – ns BLA (Chhatwal et al., 2006) BDNF KD in HPC – ns ns no drug (Heldt et al., 2007) Val66Met BDNF SNP – ns ns no drug (Soliman et al., 2010) BDNF +/- KO -*** -*** ns no drug (Psotta et al., 2013) BDNF antibody – – ns IL (Rosas-Vidal et al., 2014a) no effect no effect ns PL (Rosas-Vidal et al., 2014a) BDNF KO in forebrain ns no effect# ns PL (Choi et al., 2010) 1 drug administration following extinction training; 2 drug administration 24h prior to extinction retrieval
3 drug administration 30 min prior to extinction retrieval 4 ABA scheme, 40mg/kgMETHYLENE BLUE, NITRIC OXIDE, HISTAMINE, and LTCCs DRUG/
MANIPULATIONEXTINCTION LEARNING EXTINCTION RETRIEVAL LONG-TERM EXTINCTION ROUTE REFERENCE methylene blue ns +1 ns ip (Gonzalez-Lima & Bruchey, 2004) ns ns +1*
(Ren-A)ip (Wrubel et al., 2007) Histamine ns +1 ns CA1 (Bonini et al., 2011) dimaprit (H2 ag) ns +1 ns CA1 (Bonini et al., 2011) ranitidine (H2 ant) ns –1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns –1 ns BLA (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns –1 ns PFC (Fiorenza et al., 2012) SKF9188 (histamine methyl-transferase inhibitor) ns +1 ns CA1 (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns +1 ns BLA (Fiorenza et al., 2012) ns +1 ns PFC (Fiorenza et al., 2012) L-NAME (nNOS inhibitor)4 – – ns ip (Luo et al., 2014) CamKII-Cre Cav1.2 KO no effect ns ns no drug (McKinney et al., 2008) Cav1.3 KO no effect ns ns no drug (Busquet et al., 2008) no effect ns ns no drug (McKinney & Murphy, 2006) Nifedipine (LTCC ant) – ns ns ip (Cain et al., 2002) no effect ns ns icv (Busquet et al., 2008) no effect – ns BLA (Davis & Bauer, 2012) ns –1 ns HPC (de Carvalho Myskiw et al., 2014) Verapamil (VGCC ant) no effect – ns BLA (Davis & Bauer, 2012) - * facilitates rescue of impaired fear extinction; ** facilitates extinction of remote memories, *** only in older animals (7 months), younger ones (2 months) are not affected, # reduced fear expression at the beginning of extinction training+, improved; -, impaired; (+) or (-), only minor effects; ip, intraperitoneal injection; sc, subcutanous injection; icv, intracerebroventricular injection; ns, not studied; HPC, intra-hippocampal administration; BLA, intra-basolateral amygdala administration; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex; CA1, cornu ammonis 1; Ren-A, Fear renewal in conditioning context; SR, spontaneous recovery; Re-in, reinstatement; ag, agonist; ant, antagonist, KO, knock-out;
-
- 1
- drug administration following extinction training; 2 drug administration 24h prior extinction retrieval
- 3
- drug administration 30 min prior extinction retrieval 4 ABA scheme, 40mg/kg
-
EXTINCTION
TRAININGEXTINCTION
RETRIEVALLONGTERM
EXTINCTIONROUTE REFERENCE HDAC1 KO (HPC) ns – # ns no drug (Bahari-Javan et al., 2012) HDAC2 KO in forebrain CamKII neurons ns + *# ns no drug (Morris et al., 2013) Vorinostat/SAHA ns + ns ip (Fujita et al., 2012) ns + ** ns ip (Matsumoto et al., 2013) ns + ** ns ip (Hait et al., 2014) TSA (Trichostatin A) ns + * ns ip, HPC (Lattal et al., 2007) Sodium Butyrate – + +
(SR)ip, HPC (Stafford et al., 2012) ns + ns ip (Itzhak et al., 2012) ns + ns ip, HPC (Lattal et al., 2007) Valproic Acid – + * ns ip (Bredy et al., 2007) + * +
(Ren-A)ip (Bredy & Barad, 2008) + ** + ** +
(SR,
Ren-N)ip (Whittle et al., 2013) MS-275 (Entinostat) – + ** +
(SR,
Ren-N)ip (Whittle et al., 2013) Cl-994 ns + *** +
(SR)ip (Graff et al., 2014) FTY720 (Fingolimod) – + ** ns ip (Hait et al., 2014) - * partial extinction training: reduction of fear during the extinction training session was not to pre-conditioning levels, ** facilitates rescue of impaired fear extinction; *** facilitate remote memories combined with memory re-consolidation update paradigm; # extinction protocol based on a re-consolidation/update paradigm+, improved; -, impaired; ip, intraperitoneal injection; ns, not studied; HPC, intra-hippocampal administration; Ren-A, Fear renewal in the conditioning context; Ren-N, Fear renewal in a novel context; SR, spontaneous recovery; # reduced freezing levels at the beginning of extinction training
-
A limitation of extinction-based exposure therapy is that patients can often relapse with the passage of time, with changes in context (out of the therapy context), or under conditions of stress or other provocations, such as experiencing trauma reminders. In the parlance of learning theory, extinction memories are labile and fragile. A key goal for pharmacotherapy, therefore, is to identify compounds that overcome this fragility, by bolstering the formation, persistence and possibly context independence of extinction memories. One approach to achieving this goal is to use drugs as adjuncts to exposure therapy (cognitive enhancers) as a way to augment the extinction learning process. In this review, we discuss the various neurochemical and molecular signalling pathways that have been targeted to this end. On the one hand, there remain significant challenges to overcome, including the selective targeting of extinction-related processes without concurrent effects on original fear memories. On the other hand, there are clearly a plethora of potentially promising avenues to pursue, and we are optimistic that real advances can be made in treating trauma-related disorders.
2. Neuronal substrates of fear extinction
Using a number of complementary techniques (including electrophysiology, immediate-early gene mapping, tracing studies, lesioning/inactivation approaches and optogenetics) research is revealing the complex and interconnected brain circuitry mediating fear extinction. Several key brain areas including the amygdala (AMY), hippocampus (HPC), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), periaqueductal grey (PAG), bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and others have been implicated in extinction [for recent detailed reviews see (Myers & Davis, 2007; Ehrlich et al., 2009; Herry et al., 2010; Pape & Pare, 2010; Knapska et al., 2012; Orsini & Maren, 2012; Duvarci & Pare, 2014)]. Of these, we focus on the AMY, HPC and mPFC as major, well-defined components of the fear circuitry (see Figure 2).
While the AMY and the mPFC are crucial for the formation and maintenance of fear extinction memories, the HPC, linked with the mPFC and the AMY (Pape & Pare, 2010), processes contextual information linked with extinction (Orsini & Maren, 2012). Altering these hippocampal contributions to extinction memory or mimicking the hippocampal response within the extinction context (see below) may be mechanisms that render extinction context-independent. The AMY is a core hub in fear extinction processing. Data in rodents and humans suggest sustained AMY activity in extinction-impaired individuals, possibly resulting from a failure to engage pro-extinction circuits in cortical areas and subregions of the AMY [reviewed in (Holmes & Singewald, 2013)]. Different AMY subregions and neuronal populations have differential contributions to extinction. The centromedial AMY (CeM) is the major output station of the AMY that drives fear via its connections to the hypothalamus and brainstem regions (LeDoux et al., 1988; Fendt & Fanselow, 1999; Maren, 2001), and its responding is modulated following extinction via intra-amygdala and remote inputs. Extinction training has been shown to cause a rapid reduction of CS-evoked responses of lateral AMY (LA) neurons possibly via depotentiation of thalamic inputs (Duvarci & Pare, 2014). The basal AMY (BA) contains extinction-encoding neurons which drive GABAergic cells in the medial intercalated cell masses (ITC) and neurons in the centrolateral AMY (CeL) to inhibit the centromedial AMY (CeM) and the expression of fear (Herry et al., 2008; Duvarci & Pare, 2014). Following from these initial findings, additional studies involving optogenetic approaches, have shown that a subpopulation of the BA pyramidal neurons which express the Thy1 gene may mark the extinction neuron subpopulation (Jasnow et al., 2013).
It is also becoming apparent that different ITCs are part of a GABAergic feed-forward relay station interconnecting AMY nuclei with distinct networks within the ITCs that are engaged in particular fear stages exerting different influences on extinction (Whittle et al., 2010; Busti et al., 2011; Duvarci & Pare, 2014). Separate neuronal populations in the CeL have been found to have contrasting roles in fear and fear extinction. CeL-OFF neurons (PKCδ+ phenotype) inhibit fear-promoting CeM neurons, while CeL-ON (PKCδ- phenotype) cells stimulate fear-promoting CeM neurons (Ciocchi et al., 2010; Haubensak et al., 2010). Additionally, a very recent finding suggests that a subpopulation of neurons within the CeM –that of the Tac2 peptide-expressing cells -, is critically involved in fear learning and fear expression (Andero et al., 2014).
Another important node within the extinction circuitry is the mPFC, which shares strong interconnections with the HPC and the AMY. Fear extinction recruits the ventral segment of the mPFC, the infralimbic (IL) subdivision [the rodent correlate of the human ventromedial PFC (vmPFC)] which projects to BA extinction and ITC neurons and can thereby inhibit the activity of CeM fear output neurons [reviewed in (Amir et al., 2011; Senn et al., 2014)]. When a conditioned cue following extinction is presented in the extinction (therapy) context, the HPC activates the IL (an activation which is absent or less in a novel context), supporting subsequent CeM inhibition (Herry et al., 2010). The IL subdivision is involved in the consolidation of extinction memory and synaptic plasticity in this region and is crucial for successful extinction retrieval (Mueller & Cahill, 2010). Impaired extinction has been associated with relatively low activity in IL neurons, and successful extinction with relatively high IL neuronal activity [reviewed in (Holmes & Singewald, 2013)]. The region of the vmPFC neighboring the IL [the prelimbic cortex (PL) in rodents and the dorsal anterior cingulate in humans] plays an opposite role to the IL in regulating fear and extinction [reviewed in (Milad & Quirk, 2012)]. The PL interacts with BA fear neurons, augmenting output of the basolateral amygdaloid complex (BLA) and hence CeM activity (Senn et al., 2014).
There is now strong evidence from both animal and human studies that these various components of the neural circuit underlying extinction are functionally disturbed in individuals with impaired extinction (reviewed in (Holmes & Singewald, 2013) and importantly that successful extinction in animals (Whittle et al., 2010) and exposure therapy in humans (Hauner et al., 2012) can correct these abnormalities by triggering a lasting reorganization of this network.
Although accepted manuscripts do not have all bibliographic details available yet, they can already be cited using the year of online publication and the DOI, as follows: author(s), article title, Publication (year), DOI. Please consult the journal’s reference style for the exact appearance of these elements, abbreviation of journal names and use of punctuation.
When the final article is assigned to an volumes/issues of the Publication, the Article in Press version will be removed and the final version will appear in the associated published volumes/issues of the Publication. The date the article was first made available online will be carried over.