Canna~Fangled Abstracts

Contribution of Endocannabinoids to Intrinsic Motivation for Undirected Singing in Adult Zebra Finches

By April 12, 2022May 3rd, 2022No Comments


Journal List > Front Physiol > PMC9039130

 2022; 13: 882176.
Published online 2022 Apr 12. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2022.882176
PMCID: PMC9039130
PMID: 35492606

Associated Data

Data Availability Statement

Abstract

Songbirds, such as zebra finches, spontaneously produce many song renditions for vocal practice even in the absence of apparent recipients throughout their lives. Such “undirected singing” is driven by intrinsic motivation, which arises within individuals for internal satisfaction without immediate external rewards. Intrinsic motivation for undirected singing in adult zebra finches was previously demonstrated to be critically regulated by dopamine through D2 receptors. Here, we further investigate the mechanisms of intrinsic motivation for undirected singing by focusing on endocannabinoids, which modulate dopamine signaling and contribute to motivation and reward in mammals. In songbirds, endocannabinoids have been shown to be involved in the production of undirected songs, but whether they are involved in the intrinsic motivation for undirected singing remains unknown. Using latencies of the first song production following temporary singing suppression as a measure of intrinsic motivation for undirected singing, we demonstrate that systemic administration of the direct cannabinoid agonist WIN55212-2 decreases intrinsic motivation for singing and that those effects are largely reversed by the cannabinoid antagonist SR141716A co-administered with WIN55212-2. Administration of SR141716A alone or that of two indirect cannabinoid agonists did not significantly affect intrinsic singing motivation. These results suggest that endocannabinoids are critically involved in regulating intrinsic motivation for undirected singing and provide new insights into the neural mechanisms of intrinsically motivated motor behaviors.

Keywords: endocannabinoid, motivation, songbird, dopamine, reward, vocalization, voluntary behavior, vocal learning

Introduction

Animals, including humans, spontaneously exhibit various behaviors, even without receiving any immediate external reward such as food or money. Such voluntary behaviors are driven by intrinsic motivation and are critical for the development and optimization of cognitive, social, and physical functions throughout life (). Songbirds, such as zebra finches, offer a unique opportunity to study the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation because they spontaneously produce hundreds of renditions of stereotyped songs every day, even in the absence of apparent recipients (“undirected singing”) (Figure 1A) (). Undirected singing is thought to serve, at least in part, as a vocal practice by which birds develop and optimize song structure to prepare for future courtship activity (); other functions of undirected singing have also been suggested (EensHausberger et al., 1995; ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fphys-13-882176-g001.jpg

The cannabinoid agonist WIN significantly decreased intrinsic motivation for undirected singing and this effect was reversed by the cannabinoid antagonist SR. (A) . Spectrogram of an undirected song bout in a representative bird. (B). Schedule of lights-out (LO) and drug administration. Drugs or their vehicles were injected 30 min prior to the offset of 1-h LO periods. (C). Raster plot of song bouts produced after LO periods with vehicle (top), WIN (middle), or WIN & SR (bottom) administrations in a representative bird. (D). The effects of WIN and SR administrations on first song latencies. First song latencies were significantly increased after WIN administrations compared with those after vehicle administrations (“Veh”), and these effects were largely reversed by co-administration of SR and WIN (mean ± SEM, *p < 0.01, significance level α was corrected from 0.05 to 0.0167). Each line indicates a single bird. N.S., not significant. (E). Effects of SR administration alone on first song latencies. (F). Effects of WIN and SR administrations on initial singing rates. *p < 0.05. Conventions are as in D(G). Effects of SR administration alone on initial singing rates.

We have previously demonstrated that the intrinsic motivation for undirected singing in adult zebra finches is critically regulated by dopamine through D2 receptors (). In mammals, dopamine signaling is modulated by endocannabinoids () and they play a critical role in motivation for voluntary behaviors, such as social play and physical exercise (). In songbirds, endocannabinoids are involved in undirected singing: the amount of undirected singing is dramatically decreased by systemic administration of the direct CB1 cannabinoid agonist WIN55212-2 (WIN), and such effects are partially reversed by the CB1 cannabinoid antagonist SR141716A (SR) (). Although these results suggest that endocannabinoids play a role in the production of undirected songs, they do not necessarily indicate that endocannabinoids are directly involved in the intrinsic motivation for undirected singing. There is evidence that undirected singing acts as internal reward and is associated with a positive affective state (a state of “liking”) (). Because such singing-associated reward may facilitate subsequent song production, the inhibitory effect of WIN on undirected song production could result from a reduction in singing-associated reward rather than a direct influence of the drug on intrinsic singing motivation. In support of this view, significant correlations between singing-associated reward and endocannabinoid-related gene expression have been found in songbirds (). Thus, it remains to be determined whether the endocannabinoid system is involved in intrinsic singing motivation (a state of “wanting”) and/or singing-associated reward (a state of “liking”).

To better understand the role of endocannabinoids in undirected singing, we examined whether they are critically involved in intrinsic singing motivation. To quantify the motivation for undirected singing, we used the latency to the first song following the temporary suppression of singing (referred to as “first song latency”), a measure recently developed and described in our previous work (). Unlike the singing rate and song amount, the first song latency is not directly influenced by any process following the act of singing, allowing for examination of intrinsic singing motivation independent of singing-associated reward. We systemically administered WIN and SR as in a previous study that examined their effects on undirected song production (), and examined their effects on the first song latency to determine the role of the endocannabinoid system in intrinsic singing motivation. We additionally examined the effects of these drugs on the mean singing rates measured over a 30-min period starting at the first song after singing suppression (“initial singing rate”), which can reflect both intrinsic singing motivation and singing-associated reward (). Moreover, we assessed the effects of two indirect cannabinoid agonists, URB597 and VDM11, on undirected singing, both of which have been shown to enhance intrinsically motivated social play in adolescent rats ().

Materials and Methods

Subjects

All the subjects were adult male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata, 90–146 days post hatching). Birds were raised in our colony with their parents and siblings until ∼60 days old and then housed with their siblings and/or other male conspecifics until the experiments started. The care and treatment of animals were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of the Korea Brain Research Institute.

Song Recording and Measuring Singing Motivation

Birds were housed individually in sound-attenuating chambers (MC-050, Muromachi Kikai) under a 14:10 h light:dark cycle throughout the experiments. Undirected songs, which the birds spontaneously produced in a solo context, were recorded as previously reported (). Briefly, the output from a microphone (PRO35, Audio-Technica) positioned above the cage was amplified using a mixer (402-VLZ4, Mackie) and digitized via an audio interface (Octa-Capture UA-1010, Roland) at 44.1 kHz (16-bit). Recording was controlled by a custom-written song recording program (R. O. Tachibana at the University of Tokyo), which triggered recording if it detected four or five consecutive sound notes, each of which was defined based on the sound magnitude, duration, and intervening gap duration. Each recording ended when the silent period lasted longer than 0.5 s. Birds with sufficient singing rates (>300 song bouts per day) were used in further experiments.

Intrinsic motivation for undirected singing was quantified by measuring the latency of the first song produced following the temporal suppression of singing (“first song latency”) (). Singing was suppressed by turning off the light in the sound-attenuating chambers for 1 or 2 h depending on the drugs administered (see below), and the time interval from the offset of the lights-out (LO) period to the onset of the first song was measured. Furthermore, the mean singing rates over a 30 min period starting at the first song following LO periods (“initial singing rates”) were measured. To obtain all songs produced during those periods, all sound files recorded during those periods were screened to exclude non-song files (which include calls and/or noise) using a previously reported semi-automated method ().

Drug Administrations

For pharmacological manipulation of cannabinoid signaling, the following drugs or the corresponding vehicle were injected into the pectoral muscle: the direct CB1 cannabinoid agonist WIN (Sigma-Aldrich, W102; 1 mg/kg); selective CB1 cannabinoid inverse agonist/antagonist SR (Sigma-Aldrich, SML0800; 5 mg/kg); and the indirect endocannabinoid agonists URB597 (Sigma-Aldrich, U4133; 0.2 and 1 mg/kg) and VDM11 (Sigma-Aldrich, V3264; 1 and 5 mg/kg). The doses were selected based on previous studies (). All drugs were stored as stock solutions in DMSO at −20 °C; WIN and VDM11 were dissolved in saline before use; SR and URB597 were dissolved in 5% Tween-80/5% polyethylene glycol/saline. All birds received a single injection of a drug or vehicle every 1–2 days with a fixed LO schedule: For all drugs except URB597, birds received 1 h LO in the middle of the day (Figure 1B) and the drug or vehicle was injected 30 min before the end of the LO periods; for URB597, birds received 2 h LO period and the drug or vehicle was injected at the beginning of those LO periods, following a previous study (). Different sets of birds were used for different experiments: 8 birds were injected with WIN, the mixture of WIN and SR, and the vehicle; 13 birds were injected with SR and the vehicle; 12 birds were injected with 0.2 and 1 mg/kg of URB597 and the vehicle; 12 birds were injected with 1 and 5 mg/kg of VDM11 and the vehicle. The drugs and corresponding vehicle were injected sequentially (only a single injection per day), and those injections were repeated 2–5 times; the results (first song latency and initial singing rate) for the same drug/vehicle were averaged across injections. The temporal order of drug and vehicle injections was as follows: For the WIN and WIN + SR experiment, vehicle, WIN, and WIN + SR were injected in order but the order of WIN and WIN + SR was switched after every round of injections. For the SR alone experiment, vehicle and SR were injected alternately. For the URB597 and VDM11 experiments, vehicle, lower dose and higher dose of the drug were injected in order. We confirmed that all birds produced substantial amounts (>50 renditions) of undirected songs during the light period before each drug injection to ensure that the drug injection on the preceding day did not severely affect singing behavior on the next day.

Statistical Analysis

To examine the effects of drug administration, bird behaviors (first song latencies and initial singing rates) were compared between those occurring after drug administration and those after vehicle administration, using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. For multiple comparisons, the significance threshold (alpha) for rejecting the null hypothesis was adjusted using the Bonferroni correction. All statistical analyses were performed using the MATLAB software.

Results

The Effects of WIN and SR

Systemic administration of the cannabinoid agonist WIN at a dose that has been shown to effectively suppress undirected song production (1 mg/kg) () markedly prolonged first song latencies following temporal suppression of spontaneous undirected singing: First song latencies observed after WIN administrations were significantly longer than those after vehicle administration (Figures 1C,D; n = 8 birds, p = 0.0078, Wilcoxon signed-rank test with a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, significance level α was corrected from 0.05 to 0.0167). Moreover, this prolongation was largely reversed by the cannabinoid antagonist SR (5 mg/kg), which was co-administered with WIN (Figures 1C,D; n = 8 birds, p = 0.20 vs. vehicle). The effect of SR administration alone was assessed on first song latencies in a separate experiment, and no significant effects were observed (Figure 1E; n = 13 birds, p = 0.17, Wilcoxon signed-rank test).

The effects of these drugs on initial singing rates were also examined. Consistent with the results of the first song latencies, the initial singing rates were significantly (but marginally) reduced by WIN administration (Figure 1F; n = 8 birds, p = 0.0156), and these effects were substantially reversed by SR co-administered with WIN (Figures 1C,F, n = 8 birds, p = 0.5 vs. vehicle). In addition, no significant effects were observed after SR administration alone (Figure 1G; n = 13 birds, p = 0.15).

The Effects of URB597 and VDM11

We also examined the effects of the indirect cannabinoid agonists URB597 and VDM11 on both first song latencies and initial singing rates. For each drug, relatively low and high doses were administered, based on similar experiments in previous studies (0.2 and 1 mg/kg for URB597 and 1 and 5 mg/kg for VDM11) (). No significant effects of URB597 were found on either first song latencies (Figure 2A left; n = 12 birds, p = 1 and 0.47 for 0.2 mg/kg and 1 mg/kg, respectively) or initial singing rates (Figure 2A right; n = 12 birds, p = 0.21 and 0.76 for 0.2 mg/kg and 1 mg/kg, respectively). Similarly, no significant effects of VDM11 were observed for either first song latencies (Figure 2B left; n = 12 birds, p = 0.97 and 0.47 for 1 mg/kg and 5 mg/kg, respectively) or initial singing rates (Figure 2B right; n = 12 birds, p = 0.044 and 0.52 for 1 mg/kg and 5 mg/kg, respectively, significance level α was corrected from 0.05 to 0.0167).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is fphys-13-882176-g002.jpg

The effects of the indirect cannabinoid agonists URB597 and VDM11 on first song latencies and initial singing rates. (A). URB597 administered at 0.2 and 1 mg/kg did not have significant effects either on first song latency (left) or initial singing rate (right). Conventions are as in Figure 1D . (B) . VDM11 administered at 1 and 5 mg/kg did not have significant effects either on first song latency (left) or initial singing rate (right).

Discussion

Our results showing the marked increases in first song latencies by WIN administration and substantial reversal of those effects by SR administration suggest that endocannabinoid signaling through CB1 receptors is critically involved in the initiation of the first song after temporary singing suppression. Given that behavioral latencies have generally been used to quantify the levels of motivation for various behaviors including birdsong (), these results demonstrate that endocannabinoids critically contribute to the regulation of intrinsic motivation for undirected singing. These findings greatly advance the understanding of the role of endocannabinoids in undirected singing. A previous study using the same drugs and a similar method examined the effects of these drugs on the amount of singing (the number of song bouts over a 90-min period) and demonstrated that the singing amount was decreased by WIN and partially reversed by SR (). Since undirected song production has been suggested to act as internal reward (), which may facilitate subsequent song production, the singing amount could reflect not only singing motivation, but also singing-associated reward. Thus, the previous study measuring only singing amounts does not demonstrate whether endocannabinoids are involved in intrinsic singing motivation, singing-associated reward, or both. Because the first song latency directly reflects intrinsic singing motivation independent of singing-associated reward (), our results provide the first evidence, to our knowledge, of the significant role of endocannabinoids in intrinsic motivation for undirected singing in songbirds. The findings are consistent with those of previous studies in mammals showing that direct cannabinoid agonists, including WIN, affect the motivational aspects of both food- and drug-seeking behaviors (for review, see ).

The results of reduced initial singing rates following WIN administration also support the involvement of endocannabinoids in intrinsic singing motivation because reduced motivation for singing should result in reduced singing rates. However, these results do not eliminate the possibility that endocannabinoids are involved in singing-associated reward. As in the case of the singing amount described above, it is possible that WIN indirectly decreases initial singing rates by affecting singing-associated reward, in addition to directly affecting intrinsic singing motivation. Thus, endocannabinoids could play a role in singing-associated reward as well as in intrinsic singing motivation. In accordance with this idea, significant correlations between singing-associated reward and endocannabinoid-related gene expression have been found in songbirds (). This idea is also consistent with the roles of endocannabinoids in both motivational and pleasurable aspects of various behaviors in mammals (for review, see ).

We previously demonstrated that dopamine plays a critical role in regulating intrinsic motivation for undirected singing though D2 receptors (). Because endocannabinoids modulate the dopamine system in mammals (for review, see ), it is likely that endocannabinoids regulate intrinsic singing motivation by interacting with the dopamine system. In mammals, the interactions between endocannabinoids and the dopamine system vary across different brain areas (). For example, endocannabinoids disinhibit dopaminergic neurons in the midbrain by suppressing GABA releases from GABAergic interneurons, resulting in enhanced dopaminergic neuron activity and enhanced dopamine release from their axon terminals (). In contrast, endocannabinoids also have a function to decrease dopamine release via local actions in the striatum: dopamine release enhanced by glutamatergic inputs to the striatum is inhibited by the activation of CB1 cannabinoid receptors expressed on cortical glutamatergic terminals (). Moreover, endocannabinoids differentially interact with dopamine D1 and D2 receptors (), and these receptors are differentially involved in motivational processes (). In songbirds, it remains unclear how endocannabinoids interact with dopamine signaling to regulate singing motivation, although both dopamine-related signals and endocannabinoid-related signals are associated with the production of undirected song in many brain areas such as VTA, the medial preoptic area, and the periaqueductal gray, and the songbird basal ganglia nucleus Area X (). Identifying the neural circuits and detailed mechanisms underlying the interaction between endocannabinoids and dopamine signaling will advance our understanding of how those neuromodulators regulate singing motivation.

In contrast with the direct cannabinoid agonist WIN, which binds to CB1 cannabinoid receptors, the indirect cannabinoid agonists URB597 and VDM11 increase cannabinoid binding to receptors by preventing the breakdown or reuptake of cannabinoids, respectively. These indirect cannabinoid agonists enhance social play in rats, an intrinsically motivated rewarding behavior (). Such enhancing effects on social play are not consistent with our results of no significant effects of these drugs on undirected singing. This discrepancy could result from different mechanisms between birdsong and social play, and/or between animal species. In rodents, the effects of URB597 on social play depend on the age, strain, and behavioral context, and such effect patterns vary across different behaviors (). Given that songbirds have discrete neural circuits specialized for song learning and production (for review, see ), it is likely that the mechanisms by which endocannabinoids regulate intrinsic singing motivation differ in many aspects from mechanisms of other voluntary behaviors in other animals. Nevertheless, the tractable nature of zebra finch songs and song control circuits will enable us to understand the detailed neurophysiological mechanisms underlying intrinsic motivation for complex learned motor behaviors. Moreover, endocannabinoids in songbirds are implicated, not only in singing motivation, but also in song development (), song recognition (), and stress responses (), illustrating the importance of songbirds as a useful model system to study the roles of endocannabinoids in various biological functions.

Acknowledgments

We thank Miguel Sánchez-Valpuesta, Daisuke Mizuguchi, and Hijee Kang of the Korea Brain Research Institute for their discussions and valuable suggestions to improve this manuscript.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of the Korea Brain Research Institute.

Author Contributions

YK and SK conducted the experiments and data analyses; SK designed the experiments; YK and SK wrote the article.

Funding

This research was supported by the KBRI Basic Research Program through the Korea Brain Research Institute, funded by the Ministry of Science and ICT (Grant 21-BR-01–03 to SK).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors, and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

  • Achterberg E. J. M., van Swieten M. M. H., Driel N. V., Trezza V., Vanderschuren L. J. M. J. (2016). Dissociating the Role of Endocannabinoids in the Pleasurable and Motivational Properties of Social Play Behaviour in RatsPharmacol. Res. 110, 151–158. 10.1016/j.phrs.2016.04.031 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Berke J. D. (2018). What Does Dopamine Mean? Nat. Neurosci. 21, 787–793. 10.1038/s41593-018-0152-y [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Brainard M. S., Doupe A. J. (2001). Postlearning Consolidation of Birdsong: Stabilizing Effects of Age and Anterior Forebrain LesionsJ. Neurosci. 21, 2501–2517. 10.1523/jneurosci.21-07-02501.2001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Covey D. P., Mateo Y., Sulzer D., Cheer J. F., Lovinger D. M. (2017). Endocannabinoid Modulation of Dopamine NeurotransmissionNeuropharmacology 124, 52–61. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.04.033 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Dickens M. J., Vecchiarelli H. A., Hill M. N., Bentley G. E. (2015). Endocannabinoid Signaling in the Stress Response of Male and Female SongbirdsEndocrinology 156, 4649–4659. 10.1210/en.2015-1425 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Dunn A. M., Zann R. A. (1996). Undirected Song in Wild Zebra Finch Flocks: Contexts and Effects of Mate RemovalEthology 102, 529–539. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01145.x [CrossRef[]
  • Eens M. (1997). “Understanding the Complex Song of the European Starling: An Integrated Ethological Approach,” in Advances in the Study of Behavior. Editors Slater P.J.B., Rosenblatt J.S., Snowdon C.T., Milinski M. (Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States: Academic Press; ), 355–434. 10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60384-8 [CrossRef[]
  • El Khoury M.-A., Gorgievski V., Moutsimilli L., Giros B., Tzavara E. T. (2012). Interactions between the Cannabinoid and Dopaminergic Systems: Evidence from Animal StudiesProg. Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol. Psychiatry Cannabinoid receptor signaling modulation monoamines: implications Psychiatr. Neurol. Disord. 38, 36–50. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.12.005 [CrossRef[]
  • Gilbert M. T., Soderstrom K. (2013). Novel Song-Stimulated Dendritic Spine Formation and Arc/Arg3.1 Expression in Zebra Finch Auditory Telencephalon Are Disrupted by Cannabinoid AgonismBrain Res. 1541, 9–21. 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.10.012 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Haakenson C. M., Balthazart J., Ball G. F. (2020). Effects of Inactivation of the Periaqueductal Gray on Song Production in Testosterone-Treated Male Canaries (Serinus canaria)eNeuro 7, 1–9. 10.1523/ENEURO.0048-20.2020 [CrossRef[]
  • Hahn A. H., Merullo D. P., Spool J. A., Angyal C. S., Stevenson S. A., Riters L. V. (2017). Song-associated Reward Correlates with Endocannabinoid-Related Gene Expression in Male European Starlings ( Sturnus vulgaris )Neuroscience 346, 255–266. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.01.028 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Hahn A. H., Spool J. A., Angyal C. S., Stevenson S. A., Riters L. V. (2019). Conditioned Place Preferences Induced by Hearing Song outside the Breeding Season Relate to Neural Dopamine D1 and Cannabinoid CB1 Receptor Gene Expression in Female European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)Behav. Brain Res. 371, 111970. 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111970 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Hausberger M., Richard-Yris M.-A., Henry L., Lepage L., Schmidt I. (1995). Song Sharing Reflects the Social Organization in a Captive Group of European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris)J. Comp. Psychol. 109, 222–241. 10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.222 [CrossRef[]
  • Heimovics S. A., Cornil C. A., Ball G. F., Riters L. V. (2009). D1-like Dopamine Receptor Density in Nuclei Involved in Social Behavior Correlates with Song in a Context-dependent Fashion in Male European StarlingsNeuroscience 159, 962–973. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.042 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Heimovics S. A., Salvante K. G., Sockman K. W., Riters L. V. (2011). Individual Differences in the Motivation to Communicate Relate to Levels of Midbrain and Striatal Catecholamine Markers in Male European StarlingsHorm. Behav. 60, 529–539. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.08.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Jesse F., Riebel K. (2012). Social Facilitation of Male Song by Male and Female Conspecifics in the Zebra Finch, Taeniopygia guttata Behav. Process. 91, 262–266. 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.09.006 [CrossRef[]
  • Kim Y., Kwon S., Rajan R., Mori C., Kojima S. (2021). Intrinsic Motivation for Singing in Songbirds Is Enhanced by Temporary Singing Suppression and Regulated by DopamineSci. Rep. 11, 20350. 10.1038/s41598-021-99456-w [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Konishi M. (1965). The Role of Auditory Feedback in the Control of Vocalization in the White-Crowned Sparrow1Z. Tierpsychol 22, 770–783. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1965.tb01688.x [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Kubikova L., Wada K., Jarvis E. D. (2010). Dopamine Receptors in a Songbird BrainJ. Comp. Neurol. 518, 741–769. 10.1002/cne.22255 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Lombardino A. J., Nottebohm F. (2000). Age at Deafening Affects the Stability of Learned Song in Adult Male Zebra FinchesJ. Neurosci. 20, 5054–5064. 10.1523/jneurosci.20-13-05054.2000 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Lovinger D. M., Mateo Y., Johnson K. A., Engi S. A., Antonazzo M., Cheer J. F. (2022). Local Modulation by Presynaptic Receptors Controls Neuronal Communication and BehaviourNat. Rev. Neurosci. 28. 10.1038/s41583-022-00561-0 [CrossRef[]
  • Manduca A., Servadio M., Campolongo P., Palmery M., Trabace L., Vanderschuren L. J. M. J., et al. (2014). Strain- and Context-dependent Effects of the Anandamide Hydrolysis Inhibitor URB597 on Social Behavior in RatsEur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 24, 1337–1348. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.05.009 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Merullo D. P., Angyal C. S., Stevenson S. A., Riters L. V. (2016). Song in an Affiliative Context Relates to the Neural Expression of Dopamine- and Neurotensin-Related Genes in Male European StarlingsBrain Behav. Evol. 88, 81–92. 10.1159/000448191 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Mohebi A., Pettibone J. R., Hamid A. A., Wong J.-M. T., Vinson L. T., Patriarchi T., et al. (2019). Dissociable Dopamine Dynamics for Learning and MotivationNature 570, 65–70. 10.1038/s41586-019-1235-y [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Mooney R. (2009). Neural Mechanisms for Learned BirdsongLearn. Mem. 16, 655–669. 10.1101/lm.1065209 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Muguruza C., Redon B., Fois G. R., Hurel I., Scocard A., Nguyen C., et al. (2019). The Motivation for Exercise over Palatable Food Is Dictated by Cannabinoid Type-1 ReceptorsJCI Insight 4. 10.1172/jci.insight.126190 [CrossRef[]
  • Olivetti P. R., Balsam P. D., Simpson E. H., Kellendonk C. (2019). Emerging Roles of Striatal Dopamine D2 Receptors in Motivated Behaviour: Implications for Psychiatric DisordersBasic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 126, 47–55. 10.1111/bcpt.13271 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Parisi G. I., Kemker R., Part J. L., Kanan C., Wermter S. (2019). Continual Lifelong Learning with Neural Networks: A ReviewNeural Networks 113, 54–71. 10.1016/j.neunet.2019.01.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Parsons L. H., Hurd Y. L. (2015). Endocannabinoid Signalling in Reward and AddictionNat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 579–594. 10.1038/nrn4004 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Riters L. V., Stevenson S. A. (2012). Reward and Vocal Production: Song-Associated Place Preference in SongbirdsPhysiol. Behav. 106, 87–94. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.01.010 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Ryan R. M., Deci E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New DirectionsContemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 54–67. 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Sagheddu C., Muntoni A. L., Pistis M., Melis M. (2015). Endocannabinoid Signaling in Motivation, Reward, and AddictionInt. Rev. Neurobiol. 125, 257–302. 10.1016/bs.irn.2015.10.004 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Salamone J. D., Correa M. (2012). The Mysterious Motivational Functions of Mesolimbic DopamineNeuron 76, 470–485. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Soderstrom K., Johnson F. (2001). Zebra Finch CB1 Cannabinoid Receptor: Pharmacology and In Vivo and In Vitro Effects of ActivationJ. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 297, 189–197. [PubMed[]
  • Soderstrom K., Gilbert M. T. (2013). Cannabinoid Mitigation of Neuronal Morphological Change Important to Development and Learning: Insight from a Zebra Finch Model of PsychopharmacologyLife Sci. 92, 467–475. 10.1016/j.lfs.2012.07.031 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Soderstrom K., Johnson F. (2003). Cannabinoid Exposure Alters Learning of Zebra Finch Vocal PatternsDevelop. Brain Res. 142, 215–217. 10.1016/S0165-3806(03)00061-0 [CrossRef[]
  • Solinas M., Goldberg S. R., Piomelli D. (2008). The Endocannabinoid System in Brain Reward ProcessesBr. J. Pharmacol. 154, 369–383. 10.1038/bjp.2008.130 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Sossinka R., Böhner J. (1980). Song Types in the Zebra FinchPoephila Guttata Castanotis1Z. für Tierpsychologie 53, 123–132. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1980.tb01044.x [CrossRef[]
  • Trezza V., Vanderschuren L. J. M. J. (2008a). Bidirectional Cannabinoid Modulation of Social Behavior in Adolescent RatsPsychopharmacology 197, 217–227. 10.1007/s00213-007-1025-3 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Trezza V., Vanderschuren L. J. M. J. (2008b). Cannabinoid and Opioid Modulation of Social Play Behavior in Adolescent Rats: Differential Behavioral MechanismsEur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, 519–530. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.03.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Trezza V., Vanderschuren L. J. M. J. (2009). Divergent Effects of Anandamide Transporter Inhibitors with Different Target Selectivity on Social Play Behavior in Adolescent RatsJ. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 328, 343–350. 10.1124/jpet.108.141069 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Tumer E. C., Brainard M. S. (2007). Performance Variability Enables Adaptive Plasticity of ‘crystallized’ Adult BirdsongNature 450, 1240–1244. 10.1038/nature06390 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Vanderschuren L. J. M. J., Achterberg E. J. M., Trezza V. (2016). The Neurobiology of Social Play and its Rewarding Value in RatsNeurosci. Biobehavioral Rev. 70, 86–105. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.025 [CrossRef[]
  • Volkow N. D., Wise R. A., Baler R. (2017). The Dopamine Motive System: Implications for Drug and Food AddictionNat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 741–752. 10.1038/nrn.2017.130 [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Wang H., Lupica C. R. (2014). Release of Endogenous Cannabinoids from Ventral Tegmental Area Dopamine Neurons and the Modulation of Synaptic ProcessesProg. Neuro-Psychopharmacology Biol. Psychiatry 52, 24–27. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.01.019 [CrossRef[]
  • Wenzel J. M., Cheer J. F. (2018). Endocannabinoid Regulation of Reward and Reinforcement through Interaction with Dopamine and Endogenous Opioid SignalingNeuropsychopharmacol. 43, 103–115. 10.1038/npp.2017.126 [CrossRef[]
  • Whitney O., Soderstrom K., Johnson F. (2003). CB1 Cannabinoid Receptor Activation Inhibits a Neural Correlate of Song Recognition in an Auditory/perceptual Region of the Zebra Finch TelencephalonJ. Neurobiol. 56, 266–274. 10.1002/neu.10233 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef[]
  • Yanagihara S., Hessler N. A. (2006). Modulation of Singing-Related Activity in the Songbird Ventral Tegmental Area by Social ContextEur. J. Neurosci. 24, 3619–3627. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05228.x [PubMed] [CrossRef[]

Articles from Frontiers in Physiology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

Leave a Reply